• FREE UK delivery for orders of £20 or more
  • Order by 12pm Mon-Fri for same day dispatch

Terrain confidence and spatial awareness

by Nigel Williams

For some years now, through personal observations, I have become increasingly aware of the importance of what I term "terrain confidence" and consider it just as important as developing map and compass skills. "Confidence" is a cognitive process, influenced by experience and perceived consequences of an action. For me this represents a key difference between map reading and navigation. We often talk about confidence with navigation tools and skills, but perhaps neglect to recognise the importance of decision making and self-confidence in the terrain or environment we travel over or in. Recently published research seems to be supporting these notions.

Terrain confidence is easily observed through watching people make their early excursions onto unfamiliar surfaces or environments, such as venturing off the path into rough stony ground or through heather, or early experiences of walking on snow. We may observe it in the level of their efficiency, speed and smoothness of movement over the ground and their reduced ability to look around at the landscape and route ahead to take in valuable navigational and wayfinding information. We sometimes see it in basic route choice and decision making at a path junction where there can be a bias towards the easiest looking path which often has the least detailed navigational information. Research using eye tracking software suggests that self-confidence about our motor abilities is more likely to influence our gaze and focus, potentially drawing us into modifying our decision-making.

This is more likely with novice navigators, so exposing them to a greater range of terrain may help to broaden their motor skills and capacity to gather more useful navigation information, leading to improved decision-making. However, asking people to navigate at the same time as they experience new terrain is often a challenge, as their focus is going to be on their motor skills and where to put their feet. Furthermore, going away from a reliable handrail path can raise their concerns of becoming lost which is also distracting.

Inevitably, if on the move, our gaze is going to be limited and decisions will be made hastily. When we stop, we can observe more information. Novices often attempt to make a navigation decision by long periods of staring at their maps and limiting their gaze to the direction they are facing when they stopped and failing to take in 360º of information.

Terrain and decision-making confidence is also linked to our early years' development of spatial navigation experiences. "Sea Hero Quest" was a huge project supported by Alzheimer's UK to try to measure peoples' level of spatial navigation ability, it's decline with age, and whether it could provide an early indication of dementia. Essentially it was an on-line game which sought non-verbal spatial navigation information. It ran for nearly 5 years from 2017 in 38 countries involving 4.3 million people. There is no longer public access to the game but analysis of the data is creating many more areas of research and producing some fascinating insights which could have relevance to how we might adapt our approach to teaching navigation based on evidence rather than inherited methods.

One off shoot named "City Hero Quest" looked at the correlation between where participants grew up, gender, age and education. Not surprisingly, there is a clear correlation between our ability to navigate in environments that we are brought up in. But overall people brought up in the countryside appear to be better navigators than those brought up in a city environment.

This seems to makes sense, city dwellers are limited by the road network, their commute is often very local and their services are also close by so they generally travel less with fewer route options. Those brought up in the countryside can experience more varied terrain (making them more terrain confident) and have more short cuts and route decision-making options and have to travel further for school, work and services.

Journeying and exploring is an important aspect of developing spatial awareness for young people, it also appears that our early years' experience has the greatest and longer lasting impact on our level of spatial awareness throughout our life. (Good evidence based arguments for outdoor education).

Furthermore, there is clear evidence of differences between city dwellers brought up in cities with a clear grid pattern, often found in the US, and those brought up in a more organic city development such as older European cities. In the grid system, navigation is largely taking a 90º turn and travelling north, south or east, west. In older organically developed cities the angles of junctions, route choices and directions are much more varied.

Added to that, information analysis has also shown that on average men have a slightly better spatial awareness over women. Also, on average, people educated to tertiary level demonstrated better spatial awareness than those educated to secondary level. The gender difference may be influenced by our social norms. Sons are generally allowed or encouraged to go off and explore with their mates whereas there is a tendency to be more protective of daughters thus limiting their opportunity to explore to the same extent.

We are better at navigating in topologically similar environments to what we grew up in and early years exploration has the biggest and longest lasting influence on our spatial awareness. All this has relevance to the who, how, what, where and why of our approach to teaching the skills of navigation and the importance of confidence building. Taking a young city dweller into the countryside may need more attention to developing terrain confidence and a steadier use of teaching progressions. Terrain confidence increases our capacity to observe and gather navigational and wayfinding information and therefore supports navigation decision making. Lastly, this may also point to some of the hidden underlying reasons for the increasing number of MR callouts for walkers lost, overdue or missing.

References:
Explaining World-Wide Variation in Navigation Ability from Millions of People: Citizen Science Project Sea Hero Quest - Spiers HJ, Coutrot A, Hornberger M. 2021
Landmark-dependent Navigation Strategy Declines across the Human Life-Span: Evidence from over 37,000 Participants - Greg I. West, Patei, Antoine Coutrot, Michael Hornberger, Veronique D. Bohbot, Hugo J Spiers. 2022
Entropy of city street networks linked to future spatial navigational ability - Coutrot, A., Manley, E., Goodroe, S., Gahnstrom, C., Fiomena, G., Yesiltepo, D., Dalton, R.C., Wiener J.M., Hölsher, C., Hornberger M., Spiers, H. J. 2022
Fork in the road: How self-confidence about terrain influences gaze behaviour and path choice - Vinicius da Eira Silva and Daniel S. Marigold. 2023


You may also be interested in ...

- If you are looking for some information on the basics of navigating with a map, then take a look at Outdoor Discovery, Active Exploration with a Map - suitable for all levels of navigation, and includes several easy to follow and fun activities.




Return to the Navigation Blog