• FREE UK delivery for orders of £20 or more
  • Order by 12pm Mon-Fri for same day dispatch

How good is your sense of direction?

by Nigel Williams

Just how good is your sense of direction? I was recently asked during a webinar if it was possible to measure this. Over 20 years ago at the University of California in Santa Barbara a simple self-reporting questionnaire was developed to indicate people's environmental spatial ability. It is known as the Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale (SBSOD).

Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale (SBSOD)
You can read more about this study and test yourself - the background, the test and the scoring (it just takes 5 minutes) can be found here.

It was developed by correlating participants' self-reported sense of direction against their results of practical navigational tests and proved to be fairly accurate; it is now a widely used tool in navigation research. (On the scale of 1-7, I scored 5.6).

Our ability to make mental maps and navigate seems to be linked to our freedom and willingness to explore during the first decades of our life. In cultures where both men and women are encouraged to explore and play a part in hunting parties (for example, the Tsimane indigenous community in the Bolivian Amazon) there appears to be no difference in navigational abilities.

However, a willingness to do so must exist in the first place, and it may be that concerns for our safety or an experience or feeling of being lost when we are young (for example, panic when becoming separated from parents for a time on a crowded beach) may later deter us from having a willingness to explore. Experience and terrain confidence are key to developing our navigation ability.

There is also evidence that for people with significant spatial awareness difficulties there is a hereditary element to their poor cognitive navigation. In 2009 this became a recognised condition Developmental Topographical Disorientation sometimes referred to as directional, spatial or geographic dyslexia.

Most of us are generally good at being able to remember routes we have travelled a number of times. We can estimate the distance and time travelled and picture in our mind the sequence of landmarks that we will see along the way. The much harder ability to acquire is known as "survey knowledge". This is the ability to mentally plan short cuts between two nearby routes in our cognitive maps, being able to accurately point to known landmarks related to those routes but which are not visible from our current position, and also an ability to sketch a reasonably accurate map of the routes. Testing route and survey knowledge can provide a measure of our ability to create accurate mental maps.

Further research has highlighted the detrimental impact of habitual GPS use, which would seem to bypass the development of survey knowledge. There is a direct correlation between GPS use and people's spatial awareness and this research has been carried out with the same participants over a number of years. The heaviest users showed the worst spatial awareness and a greater deterioration of it over time by comparison to the occasional users.

The GPS draws our attention away from landmark observation, absorbing the relationships between landmarks and our relative position to them as we travel. It also removes our decision making and learning from errors which often leads to further exploration, even though it may be unwelcome at the time. With an in-car GPS our navigation is more based on auditory information instead of observational information which is not how we navigate in the outdoors.

The GPS is a really useful navigational tool, the issue is when and how we use it in conjunction with our cognitive navigation and more traditional tools of map and compass.




Return to the Navigation Blog